Recognizing Young Children’s Anxiety While Lying: Utilizing Heart Rate Data
From a Smart Bracelet
Author: Shu-hui Chiu (Department of Guidance & Counseling, National Changhua University of Education; Department of Early Childhood Education, National Taichung University of Education), Tsu-Te Chuang Hu (Department of Education, University of Taipei)
Vol.&No.:Vol. 70, No. 1
Date:March 2025
Pages:115-141
DOI:https://doi.org/10.6209/JORIES.202503_70(1).0004
Abstract:
Motivation and Purpose
Preschool children and those with special needs often struggle to articulate their anxiety clearly. Identifying anxiety through a simple, non-invasive, and valid physiological indicator would be invaluable in educational, counseling, and academic research fields. This study aims to propose practical applications such as using a smart bracelet with alert settings for children with emotional regulation difficulties. The monitors could warn teachers or the children themselves when heart rates exceed a specific threshold, indicating potential emotional distress. This study compares the heart rate variations of preschool children across different phases of an experiment. The primary objectives are: (1) to investigate the heart rate changes in lying versus honest children during the four experimental phases (baseline, play, alone, response), and (2) to utilize data from heart rate monitors to establish threshold values, distinguishing between calm (baseline) and anxious (alone, response) states.
Literature Review
The Polyvagal Theory describes how the autonomic nervous system responds to various environmental contexts, influencing subsequent physiological responses such as heart rate (Porges, 2009). When environmental threats increase, the ventral vagal complex is suppressed, leading to the dominance of the sympathetic nervous system, resulting in an accelerated heart rate. Children as young as three and a half years old begin to lie (Evans & Lee, 2011). In experimental settings, spontaneous lying behaviors in children are often studied through a temptation scenario, such as leaving the child alone after instructing them not to peek at a noise-making toy (Lewis et al., 1989) or to refrain from looking at a covered card to win a game (Zhao et al., 2017). Lying can induce anxiety and heart rate changes, with heart rate variability serving as an indicator of anxiety levels.
Methodology
This study invited 91 children (45 girls and 46 boys) aged 5.43 to 6.49 years (M = 5.93) to participate in a game. Based on the card game scenario from Zhao et al. (2017), the experimental setup was divided into four phases: baseline (sitting quietly before the game), play (introduction of the game), alone (researcher leaves the room), and response (researcher asks if the child peeked at the cards). Children were categorized into lying and honest groups based on their spontaneous behavior.
Research Results
1. Overall Heart Rate Variability: The heart rate variability was significantly higher during the alone and response phases compared to the play and baseline phases. The findings support the feasibility of using heart rate to detect anxiety in young children.
2. Distinguishing Lying and Honest Groups: The heart rate variations in different phases could not reliably distinguish between lying and honest children. This could be due to greater within-subject variability in heart rates than between-group differences or some honest children experiencing anxiety in the later phases.
3. Individual Heart Rate Comparison: Comparing a child’s heart rate against their baseline average and standard deviation is essential to determine anxiety states. This supports the notion that heart rate and its variability are influenced by individual physiological traits (Meijer & Verschuere, 2018). The variability in baseline heart rates predicted subsequent heart rate changes, with greater baseline variability indicating a higher likelihood of anxiety-induced heart rate increases.
4. Effect Sizes of Phase and Baseline Heart Rate: Both the phase factor and the baseline heart rate standard deviation (as a covariate) had small effect sizes on subsequent heart rates. The effect size of the phase (representing context) was slightly higher than that of the covariate (representing physiological traits). This suggests that situational factors have a more substantial impact on heart rate changes than personal traits in this study.
5. Maximum Heart Rate Variability: The highest heart rate variability during the alone phase distinguished between the honest and lying groups. Honest children showed greater variability, possibly due to internal struggles and anxiety while deciding whether to peek, or because some waited calmly without intent to cheat. The lying group was more uniformly anxious about deciding to cheat.
6. Minimum Heart Rate: Minimum heart rates showed significant differences across all phases, indicating a consistent upward trend. According to The Polyvagal Theory (Porges, 1995), as anxiety increases, the minimum heart rate stabilizes and rises unless controlled by the dorsal vagal complex, which lowers the heart rate. Therefore, examining maximum and minimum heart rates, in addition to the average, provides richer information.
7. Adjusting Heart Rate Alert Thresholds: Results indicated that using the formula “baseline maximum heart rate + 0.2 * (baseline maximum heart rate - baseline average)” produced similar effects to Lee et al.’s (2010) “baseline average + 2SD” but both were slightly lenient. In practical applications, thresholds can be flexibly adjusted according to individual children’s traits or specific usage purposes, tailoring appropriate alert values for each child.
Discussion and Recommendations
This study employed an experimental setting to tempt children to lie. The findings show that children’s heart rates significantly increased when they had the opportunity to cheat and subsequently lie to cover up their actions, compared to their calm states. This demonstrates the practical utility of using heart rate changes to detect emotional shifts in children. Additionally, the study found that real-time data from heart rate bracelets, including the average and maximum heart rates during calm states, can be used to set alert thresholds with similar effectiveness to the standards used by Lee et al. (2010).
Exploring the use of heart rate to detect anxiety can expand its application range. For example, during play therapy with traumatized children, monitoring their emotional fluctuations with heart rate bracelets can be beneficial. Similarly, pairing heart rate bracelets with behavioral training techniques for children with emotional disorders can help manage anxiety. Setting alert thresholds on the bracelets can teach children to pause activities and use pre-taught emotional regulation strategies when alerted. It can also prompt caregivers to identify environmental factors that may cause anxiety, enabling preventive measures.
Most children’s heart rates did not significantly spike, indicating that the experimental setting induced only mild anxiety. However, in practical applications, severe anxiety might cause the dorsal vagal complex to take over the autonomic nervous system, leading to a sudden drop in heart rate. Therefore, setting minimum heart rate thresholds is also crucial. Lee et al. (2010) used the baseline average heart rate minus two standard deviations as an alert threshold. While this study’s data do not provide specific recommendations for this, practitioners can experiment with this approach. Additionally, the experience of lying might predict heart rate changes across different phases, a variable not considered in this study due to a lack of relevant data. Future research should include this variable. Lastly, factors other than anxiety, such as excitement, can also increase heart rates, which may explain the significant rise in heart rates during the play phase in this study.
Keywords:heart rate, smart bracelet, anxiety, lying, preschoolers
《Full Text》
References:Craig, D.(2017)。國際犯罪學專家教你分辨真偽、立破謊言(張瓅文,譯)。商周。(原著出版於2012年)
【Craig, D. (2017). How to become a human lie-detector in under 60 minutes (L.-W. Chang, Trans.). Business Weekly. (Original work published 2012)】
黃秋華、鄭芬蘭、楊晴閔(2006)。不同性別幼兒欺騙動機及其行為之實驗研究。國民教育研究學報,16,195-223。
【Huang, C.-H., Zheng, F.-L., & Yang, Q.-M. (2006). An experimental study of gender different in children’s deception motivation and behavior. Journal of Research on Elementary and Secondary Education, 16, 195-223.】
莊胡祖德、邱淑惠(2023)。說謊與認知彈性:幼兒頭腦靈活有助說謊?教育學報,51(2),187-205。
【Chuang Hu, T.-T., & Chiu, S. (2023). Deception and cognitive flexibility: Does being smart help young children lying? Education Journal, 51(2), 187-205.】
張欣戊(1998)。騙與受騙:學前兒童的欺瞞能力。中華心理學刊,40(1),1-13。
【Chang, H.-W. (1998). Preschoolers’ deceptive ability. Chinese Journal of Psychology, 40(1), 1-13.】
» More
一、中文文獻
Craig, D.(2017)。國際犯罪學專家教你分辨真偽、立破謊言(張瓅文,譯)。商周。(原著出版於2012年)
【Craig, D. (2017). How to become a human lie-detector in under 60 minutes (L.-W. Chang, Trans.). Business Weekly. (Original work published 2012)】
黃秋華、鄭芬蘭、楊晴閔(2006)。不同性別幼兒欺騙動機及其行為之實驗研究。國民教育研究學報,16,195-223。
【Huang, C.-H., Zheng, F.-L., & Yang, Q.-M. (2006). An experimental study of gender different in children’s deception motivation and behavior. Journal of Research on Elementary and Secondary Education, 16, 195-223.】
莊胡祖德、邱淑惠(2023)。說謊與認知彈性:幼兒頭腦靈活有助說謊?教育學報,51(2),187-205。
【Chuang Hu, T.-T., & Chiu, S. (2023). Deception and cognitive flexibility: Does being smart help young children lying? Education Journal, 51(2), 187-205.】
張欣戊(1998)。騙與受騙:學前兒童的欺瞞能力。中華心理學刊,40(1),1-13。
【Chang, H.-W. (1998). Preschoolers’ deceptive ability. Chinese Journal of Psychology, 40(1), 1-13.】
二、外文文獻
Akar, S. A., Kara, S., Agambayev, S., & Bilgiç, V. (2015). Nonlinear analysis of EEGs of patients with major depression during different emotional states. Computers in Biology and Medicine, 67, 49-60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compbiomed.2015.09.019
Bailenson, J. N., Pontikakis, E. D., Mauss, I. B., Gross, J. J., Jabon, M. E., Hutcherson, C. A., Nass, C., & John, O. (2008). Real-time classification of evoked emotions using facial feature tracking and physiological responses. International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 66(5), 303-317. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijhcs.2007.10.011
Bowen, M. (1978). Family therapy in clinical practice. Rowman & Littlefield.
Brown, C. L., Van Doren, N., Ford, B. Q., Mauss, I. B., Sze, J. W., & Levenson, R. W. (2020). Coherence between subjective experience and physiology in emotion: Individual differences and implications for well-being. Emotion, 20(5), 818-829. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000579
Chen, K.-H., Brown, C. L., Wells, J. L., Rothwell, E. S., Otero, M. C., Levenson, R. W., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2021). Physiological linkage during shared positive and shared negative emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 121(5), 1029-1056. https://doi.org/ 10.1037/pspi0000337
Cohen, J. (1992). Statistical power analysis. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 1(3), 98-101. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.ep10768783
El-Sheikh, M., & Harger, J. (2001). Appraisals of marital conflict and children’s adjustment, health, and physiological reactivity. Developmental Psychology, 37(6), 875-885. https://doi.org/ 10.1037/0012-1649.37.6.875
El-Sheikh, M., Harger, J., & Whitson, S. M. (2001). Exposure to interparental conflict and children’s adjustment and physical health: The moderating role of vagal tone. Child Development, 72(6), 1617-1636. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00369
El-Sheikh, M., Keller, P. S., & Erath, S. A. (2007). Marital conflict and risk for child maladjustment over time: Skin conductance level reactivity as a vulnerability factor. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 35(5), 715-727. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-007-9127-2
Evans, A. D., & Lee, K. (2011). Verbal deception from late childhood to middle adolescence and its relation to executive functioning skills. Developmental Psychology, 47(4), 1108-1116. https:// doi.org/10.1037/a0023425
Gödert, H. W., Rill, H. G., & Vossel, G. (2001). Psychophysiological differentiation of deception: The effects of electrodermal lability and mode of responding on skin conductance and heart rate. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 40(1), 61-75. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167- 8760(00)00149-5
Kerr, M. E., & Bowen, M. (1988). Family Evaluation: An approach based on Bowen theory. W. W. Norton.
Lee, W. Y., Ng, M. L., Cheung, B. K. L., & Yung, J. W. (2010). Capturing children’s response to parental conflict and making use of it. Family Process, 49(1), 43-58. https://doi.org/10.1111/ j.1545-5300.2010.01307.x
Lewis, M. (1993). The development of deception. In M. Lewis & C. Saarni (Eds.), Lying and deception in everyday life (pp. 90-105). The Guilford Press. https://psycnet.apa.org/record/ 1993-97532-003
Lewis, M., Stranger, C., & Sullivan, M. W. (1989). Deception in 3-year-olds. Developmental Psychology, 25(3), 439-443. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.25.3.439
Mauss, I. B., Levenson, R. W., McCarter, L., Wilhelm, F. H., & Gross, J. J. (2005). The tie that binds? Coherence among emotion experience, behavior, and physiology. Emotion, 5(2), 175-190. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.5.2.175
Minuchin, S., Rosman, B. L., & Baker, L. (1978). Psychosomatic families: Anorexia nervosa in context. Harvard University Press. https://doi.org/10.4159/harvard.9780674418233
Meijer, E. H., & Verschuere, B. (2018). Detection deception using psychophysiological and neural measures. In H. Otgaar & M. L. Howe (Eds.), Finding the truth in the courtroom: Dealing with deception, lies, and memories (pp. 209-224). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/ oso/9780190612016.001.0001
Peskin, J. (1992). Ruse and representations: On children’s ability to conceal information. Developmental Psychology, 28(1), 84-89. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.28.1.84
Polak, A., & Harris, P. L. (1999). Deception by young children following noncompliance. Developmental Psychology, 35(2), 561-568. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.35.2.561
Popliger, M., Talwar, V., & Crossman, A. (2011). Predictors of children’s prosocial lie-telling: Motivation, socialization variables, and moral understanding. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110(3), 373-392. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2011.05.003
Porges, S. W. (1995). Orienting in a defensive world: Mammalian modifications of our evolutionary heritage: A polyvagal theory. Psychophysiology, 32(4), 301-318. https://doi.org/10.1111/ j.1469-8986.1995.tb01213.x
Porges, S. W. (2009). The polyvagal theory: New insights into adaptive reactions of the autonomic nervous system. Cleveland Clinic Journal of Medicine, 76(Suppl 2), S86-S90. https://doi.org/ 10.3949/ccjm.76.s2.17
Shu, L., Yu, Y., Chen, W., Hua, H., Li, Q., Jin, J., & Xu, X. (2020). Wearable emotion recognition using heart rate data from a smart bracelet. Sensors, 20(3), 718. https://doi.org/10.3390/ s20030718
Talwar, V., & Lee, K. (2002). Development of lying to conceal a transgression: Children’s control of expressive behavior during verbal deception. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 26(5), 436-444. https://doi.org/10.1080/01650250143000373
Talwar, V., Gordon, H. M., & Lee, K. (2007). Lying in the elementary school years: Verbal deception and its relation to second-order belief understanding. Developmental Psychology, 43(3), 804-810. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.3.804
Wang, L., & Wang, Z. (2019). The modified temptation resistance task: A paradigm to elicit children’s strategic lie-telling. Journal of Visualized Experiments, 134, Article 57189. https:// doi.org/10.3791/57189
Zhao, L., Heyman, G. D., Chen, L., & Lee, K. (2017). Praising young children for being smart promotes cheating. Psychological Science, 28(12), 1868-1870. https://doi.org/10.1177/ 0956797617721529